cummings02-QualityOfOnlineRelationships.pdf

(105 KB) Pobierz
Kraut_ lo
Q UALITY OF
Online
Social Relationships
O NLINE RELATIONSHIPS ARE LESS VALUABLE
THAN OFFLINE ONES . I NDEED , THEIR
NET BENEFIT DEPENDS ON WHETHER THEY
SUPPLEMENT OR SUBSTITUTE FOR
OFFLINE SOCIAL RELATIONSHIPS .
P
{ By Jonathon N. Cummings, Brian Butler, and Robert Kraut }
eople use the Internet
intensely for interper-
sonal communication,
sending and receiving
email, contacting
friends and family via
instant messaging ser-
vices, visiting chat rooms,
or subscribing to distribution lists,
among other activities. The evidence is
clear that interpersonal communication
is an important use of the Internet, if not
its most important use. For example,
both self-report surveys [12] and
computer monitoring studies [5] indicate that email
is the most popular online application.
Claims regarding the Internet’s usefulness for
developing social relationships, however, remain con-
troversial. Both personal testimonials (for example,
[10]) and systematically collected data document the
deep and meaningful social relationships people can
cultivate online (for example, [8]).
This evidence, however, conflicts with data com-
paring the value that people place on their online rela-
tionships with offline relationships and with data
comparing social relationships among heavy and light
Internet users. For example, Parks and Roberts [9]
surveyed users of multiplayer environments called
MOOs. Ninety-three percent of the users had made
friends online, but when asked to compare their
online friendships with those offline, respondents
rated offline ones higher. Respondents to Nie’s [7]
national survey reported spending less time with
friends and family since going online, with the decline
greatest among the most frequent Internet users. And
Kraut et al. [6] presented longitudinal evidence to
demonstrate that among new Internet users, online
time diminished social involvement and psychologi-
cal well being.
Understanding the impact of the Internet on
human social relationships requires two types of evi-
dence. First, we need to know how computer-medi-
ated communication affects the quality of particular
social interactions and relationships. Are the online
ones better, the same, or worse than those sustained
by other means? Second, we need to know how com-
puter-mediated communication affects one’s mix of
social interactions and relationships. The impact of
the Internet is likely to be very different if it supple-
ments communication with existing friends and fam-
ily, or if instead it substitutes for more traditional
communication and social ties.
This article addresses the first question by explicitly
comparing online and offline social interaction. We
briefly summarize evidence from several empirical
studies, all of which suggest that computer-mediated
communication, and in particular email, is less valu-
able for building and sustaining close social relation-
COMMUNICATIONS OF THE ACM July 2002/Vol. 45, No. 7
103
T HE
72808962.003.png
ships than face-to-face contact and telephone conver-
sations. These studies include the following surveys:
3.0
2.5
• International bank employees who describe the
value of particular communication sessions for
work relationships;
• College students, using the same methodology,
but focusing on personal relationships;
• A longitudinal study of new Internet users; and
• Examination of behavior on email-based listservs.
2.0
1.5
1.0
Work
Relationships
Face-to-face
Phone
Email
tion, one might also object
that personal relationships are
not central to work activity,
although many studies stress
their importance for getting
work done. To counter these
objections, we replicated the original study in 1999
among students at an eastern university. These stu-
Comparing Communication Over
Different Media
One way to evaluate the usefulness of the Internet
for developing and maintaining social ties is to ask
people to compare particular communication ses-
sions on relevant outcomes. One can then relate the
outcomes to features of the communication session
(for example, who it was with, the duration, and the
modality over which it occurred). This technique
has been used to uncover features of conversation
that lead to the development of social relationships
in face-to-face settings (for example, [3]). We apply
it to email, telephone, and face-to-face communica-
tion among bank employees and university students.
In our 1991 study, 979 employees of a multina-
tional bank reported on their most recent communi-
cation conducted over different media. About 81%
used email in their jobs, sending an average of 15
messages per week. Respondents evaluated the useful-
ness of communication episodes using criteria related
to the success of work groups, including usefulness for
getting work done and for developing or sustaining a
work relationship, utilizing a 3-point scale, where 1
meant not very useful and 3 meant very useful. We
report data on 5,205 communication episodes that
occurred in person, by telephone, or by email (see [4]
for more detail.)
Figure 1 compares the evaluation of conversations
held over each media for the two measures of success.
Respondents reported communication by email to be
reliably worse than communication conducted face-
to-face or by telephone, both for getting work done
and for sustaining work relationships. However, the
disadvantages of email were significantly greater for
maintaining relationships than for getting work
done. These differences among the media remain
even when one statistically controls for relevant vari-
ables, including respondents’ gender, age, job title,
daily volume of communication, and experience
with email.
One might object that this data comes from the
early years of email, although employees in this firm
had been using email since the mid-1970s. In addi-
Figure 1. Perceived
usefulness of commu-
nication by media and
outcomes in an
international bank.
3.0
2.5
2.0
1.5
1.0
Work
Information
Relationships
Face-to-face
Phone
Email
dents used email extensively,
estimating a mean of 11 mes-
sages per day, and were in a
stage in life that stressed the
importance of developing per-
sonal relationships. Some 39
students completed a diary, recording information
about each of 259 communication episodes in which
they had participated during a four-hour block—late
afternoon to early evening. Students recorded their
relationship with their communication partner (rela-
tive, friend, acquaintance, or other), its duration, the
topic of conversation (schoolwork, personal, or
other), and the modality over which it occurred.
Respondents evaluated each communication for its
usefulness in getting work done, exchanging infor-
mation, and developing or maintaining a personal
relationship. They made their evaluations on 5-point
scale.
Like the banking study, students evaluated email
communication sessions as an inferior means to
maintaining personal relationships compared to those
conducted in person (p <0.05) and by telephone (p <
0.05), these latter being equal (see Figure 2). The stu-
dents, however, found email to be as good as the tele-
phone and in-person communication for completing
Figure 2. Perceived
usefulness of com-
munication by media
and outcome among
college students.
104
July 2002/Vol. 45, No. 7 COMMUNICATIONS OF THE ACM
72808962.004.png
schoolwork (p > 0.10), and even better for the
exchange of information (p < 0.05).
Students also estimated the frequency of commu-
nication over the different modalities and the strength
of their relationship with each of the 148 partners. We
created an index of relationship strength by averaging
their answers to two questions: “How close do you
feel to this person?” and “How often do you get favors
or advice from this person “(alpha = 0.92). We used
linear regression to predict the strength of the rela-
tionship from frequency of communication with that
partner over the different modalities: email, in-person,
and telephone. Frequency of communication across
all three modalities was significantly related to the
strength of relationship, both directly and once the
partner’s gender, nature of the relations, length of the
relationship, and geographic distance between the
parties were controlled statistically. However, commu-
munication. The first, whom we refer to as the “Inter-
net partner,” was the individual outside of their
household to whom they sent the most email, as
recorded in computer-generated usage logs collected
as part of the project. Some 111 respondents
answered questions about an Internet partner. The
second, whom we refer to as the “nonInternet part-
ner,” was the person outside of their household with
whom respondents claimed to have the most frequent
communication in any modality. Some 125 respon-
dents answered questions about a nonInternet part-
ner. To allow for comparisons between relationships
conducted by email and those conducted primarily
over other modalities, we limit our analyses here to
the 99 respondents who answered questions both
about an Internet and a nonInternet partner, and for
whom these partners were different individuals.
Respondents indicated each partner’s gender and
age, duration of acquaintance, role relation (for
example, family, friend, co-worker), and geographic
proximity (for example, neighborhood, city, state).
Participants then rated their frequency of email, face-
to-face, and telephone communication: (5-daily, 4-
weekly, 3-monthly, 2-less often, 1-never). A 5-point
scale indicated psychological closeness with the part-
ner: “I feel very close,” “I could freely confide in this
person,” “This person is important to me,” and “I
understand this person fully” (alpha = 0.90).
We were interested in three questions: Do people
differ in the overall volume of communication they
have with the people they keep up with using differ-
ent modalities? Do they differ in how close they feel
toward them? Is communication with a partner over
different modalities predictive of differing degrees of
psychological closeness?
The number of respondents’ communication ses-
sions per month, broken and summed over all modal-
ities, indicate that participants communicated less
frequently with their Internet partner (5.2
times/month) than their nonInternet partner (7.2
times/month, p < 0.001) as shown in Figure 3.
Although respondents communicated more by email
with their Internet partner (p < 0.001), they commu-
nicated less using the other modalities (p < 0.001 for
face-to-face and p < 0.001 for telephone). Respon-
dents also reported feeling less close to their Internet
partner than to their nonInternet partner (p < 0.001).
Using a least squares regression analysis, we pre-
dicted psychological closeness from frequency of com-
munication for the nonInternet partner and Internet
partner, controlling for sex, age, role relation, duration
of acquaintance, and physical proximity. Most
notably, frequency of communication was a critical
predictor of psychological closeness with the nonIn-
8
6
4
2
0
NonInternet partner
Internet partner
Face-to-face
Phone
Email
Total
nicating in person
(Beta s= 0.36) and by
telephone (Beta = 0.27)
were both significantly
better predictors of a strong relationship than was
communication by email (Beta = 0.15).
Comparing Internet Versus NonInternet
Social Partners
In these studies, respondents selected communica-
tion episodes and partners based on the recency of
the communication session. This procedure has the
advantage of sampling all potential conversations,
but may over-represent social relationships not
important to the respondents, but are frequent sim-
ply because the partners are nearby. Here, we com-
pare the value of using computer-mediated and
noncomputer-mediated communication to keep up
with partners with whom the respondents have a
substantial amount of communication. The data
comes from the HomeNet project, a field trial that
tracked Internet usage and communication behavior
among a sample of 93 households in Pittsburgh dur-
ing their first year or two online (see [5]).
Participants answered a series of questions about
two individuals with whom they had frequent com-
COMMUNICATIONS OF THE ACM July 2002/Vol. 45, No. 7
105
Figure 3. Frequency of
communication per month by
modality and partner type.
72808962.005.png
100%
100%
75%
75%
50%
50%
25%
25%
0%
500
1000
1500
2000
0%
0%
25%
Ratio of Active Participants to Total Members
75%
100%
Membership Size
A. (N=204 groups)
B. (N=139 groups)
100%
100%
75%
75%
50%
50%
25%
25%
0%
5
10
15
20
25
0%
1.0
1.5 2.0 2.5 3.0 3.5 4.0
Average Number of Messages per Discussion Thread
Average Number of Messages Per Day
C. (N=204 groups)
D. (N=139 groups)
ternet partner
(Beta = 0.40),
but not with the
Internet partner
(Beta = -0.08).
The difference is statistically significant (p < 0.001).
The weaker association of communication with close-
ness for the Internet sample is analogous to findings
from the student sample.
Social relationships offline involve more commu-
nication than those developed online, and thus pre-
dicted psychological closeness. Given our
cross-sectional data, we cannot tell if communication
does not lead to closeness when people are communi-
cating electronically, or if people are exchanging email
with people to whom they do not feel close. In either
case, they are not getting as much social benefit from
email as they do from their other communication
activity.
these electronic or virtual communities are often
described as groups where relationships form, and
whose members provide each other with compan-
ionship, information, and social support [11].
While existing studies and stories of electronic
groups provide insight into the types of social activity
that can occur in electronic collectives, the anecdotal
nature of this research leaves open the question of
what typically happens. Are active, tightly knit elec-
tronic groups, in which people form personal rela-
tionships and develop a sense of belonging, the norm
or are the cases reported in the literature interesting
exceptions? To examine this question, we collected
data from a sample of 204 Internet listservs. The data
shows that, on average, listservs are much more like
loosely knit, voluntary organizations than the tightly
knit social communities highlighted in prior case
studies.
The sample consisted of 204 unmanaged and
unmoderated email-based listservs, drawn from a
population of approximately 70,000 listservs. An ini-
tial random sample of 1,066 was stratified by topic
type (work-related, personal, and mixed) to ensure it
included a range of topics and member populations.
Listservs were dropped from the initial sample if the
list owner declined to participate in the study (21%);
the listserv was defunct (16%); it had closed mem-
bership, generally as part of an organization, course,
or task force (15%); or it could not provide member-
ship data in an automated fashion. The final sample
consisted of lists evenly divided among those oriented
Online Social Groups
The research we described so far concentrates on
dyadic relationships between individuals in their
online and offline lives. Yet one of the prominent
features of the Internet is the presence of larger
social collectives, which researchers have called “elec-
tronic groups” or “communities.” Even before the
advent of the Web, the Internet provided an infra-
structure for online group-level social behavior,
through USENET and email-based distribution
lists. In descriptions of social life on the Internet,
106
July 2002/Vol. 45, No. 7 COMMUNICATIONS OF THE ACM
50%
Figure 4. The distribution of listservs
in membership, and participation
ratio, message volume, and discussion
length.
72808962.006.png 72808962.001.png
around professional, personal,
and academic topics. Based on
descriptions of the lists, we were
able to classify them as purely
electronic or as hybrid, combin-
ing both electronic and tradi-
tional communications, especially
conventional face-to-face meet-
ings. For example, a national list
for youth hockey was judged as
purely electronic, while the mail-
ing list for a city-specific country
dancers’ group was judged as
hybrid (see [2]).
For a 130-day period we col-
lected data on each listserv’s
membership and communica-
tion activity. During the observa-
tion period, membership was
characterized in terms of size (number of members),
growth (members entering as a percentage of initial
size), loss (members leaving as a percentage of initial
size), and net change in size (as a percentage of initial
size). Communication activity was measured in terms
of volume (number of messages per day) and interac-
tivity (length of discussion threads). In addition, mea-
sures of member participation (percentage of
members contributing messages and the concentra-
tion of message contributions among the active par-
ticipants) were created for each listserv. The table
appearing here delineates this information, and con-
trasts the purely electronic with the hybrid lists. Fig-
ures 4a–d, respectively, show the distribution of the
number of subscribers, the proportion of all sub-
scribers who posted a least one message during the
130-day observation period, the daily number of mes-
sages per listserv, and the average number of messages
per discussion thread.
Unlike traditional small groups, listervs have large,
fluctuating memberships in which a small core of
active participants generates relatively low levels of
sporadic communication, whose messages rarely
receive a response. Small groups, as described in the
social psychological research literature, have between
3 and 15 members, with relatively low turnover. By
comparison, the listservs were much larger (median of
64 members), with high churn (22% of original
members dropping out annually and double this
number joining). In contrast to highly interactive
conversation involving almost all group members
(typical of small groups), listservs exhibit little com-
munication, with a full 33% exhibiting no communi-
cation during the 130-day observation period. Of
those that did, the median listerv accrued 0.28 mes-
Variable
Measure
Mean
Overall
Median
Overall
Median
Pure
Electronic
Lists
Median
Hybrid
Lists
Different
Pure vs.
Hybrid
Size
Number of members at start of
observation period
Member entering during 130
day observation period as
percentage of initial size
Members leaving during 130
day observation period as
percentage of initial size
Net change in size as
percentage of initial size
Average number of messages
per day
Average discussion thread length.
Thread length of 1 indicates the
originating message
Percentage of members who
contributed messages during the
observation period
163
64
91
34
***
Growth
23%
15%
14.8
16.7
Loss
13%
8%
8.1%
8.1%
Change
9%
4%
4%
2%
Volume
A
1.650
0.28
0.24
0.38
Interactivity
1.580
1.33
1.61
1.50
Member
Participation
22%
15%
15%
19%
N = 204 listservs
*** p< 0.001
33% of the sampled groups had no activity during the observation period. The reported data is from the active groups.
sages per day (or less than
0.0004 messages per sub-
scriber per day). Over
50% of members con-
tributed no messages over the 130-day observation
period, and a small number generated most of the
messages. Conversation was not interactive. On aver-
age, fewer than one message out of three received any
response.
The hybrid groups differed little from the purely
electronic groups. Though they were significantly
smaller, probably reflecting the more limited geo-
graphic area from which they could attract members,
both types of groups had similar high turnover, low
volume of messages, low level of interactivity, and
domination by a small proportion of their member-
ship. Regardless of how the hybrid groups acted when
they met face-to-face, online they acted like typical
weak-tie collectives.
In terms of membership size and change, commu-
nication volume and structure, and participation lev-
els, Internet listservs do not appear to be intimate
social groups. These findings highlight a bias in prior
research on online social activity. While the goal of
describing the existence of true social behaviors in
online environments has been well served by focusing
on highly active and interactive examples of electronic
collectives, these cases are not representative of what
typically happens. For example, interactivity is a com-
mon theme in many descriptions of online social
activity (for example, [1]). However, our results imply
that while interactivity can occur in these contexts, it
is the exception, not the rule, when it occurs.
It was not the case that all listservs in this sample
had impoverished social behavior, although this was
Population characteristics
for Internet listservs.
COMMUNICATIONS OF THE ACM July 2002/Vol. 45, No. 7
107
A
72808962.002.png
Zgłoś jeśli naruszono regulamin